Unanticipated Animals: What Happens When Pets Appear in Research Interviews?
A new study finds pets are often written
out of research reports.
People are not anthropomorphizing the animals, according to Ryan and Ziebland. They say, “the narratives show more of a levelling between the status of the pet and person that is often not recognized or acknowledged in popular discourses around animal ownership.”
The paper is based on what’s called a ‘secondary analysis’ of an existing dataset of interviews. They looked at 231 interviews with people with autism, Parkinson’s disease, stroke or heart failure, and carers of people with dementia or multiple sclerosis. The interviews were all conducted as part of research into aspects of those conditions – and notably the focus was not on pets.
The transcripts were studied for places where pets appeared. Anywhere that an ‘interruption’ was listed they went to the audio recordings to see what happened. In some cases the interruption was a pet, in which case they transcribed this in full. Then they analyzed all the places where pets were mentioned or made a noise.
You might also like:
We all know the saying “never work with children or animals”. Normally it applies to actors. But what happens when a researcher goes to
interview someone and a pet is there too? A new paper by Sara Ryan
and Sue Ziebland
(University of Oxford) says that health scientists
are not paying enough attention to the importance of pets in people’s lives.
Their analysis shows that pets are often
ignored or are seen as an interruption in interviews. In one case, someone talks about how
their diagnosis with a serious health condition was difficult, especially
because they did not feel the doctor listened to them as a patient. The
researcher’s response: “Can I shut that cat up?” (Fortunately the video of the
interview showed this was a friendly interaction).
Although this is the most striking example
given in the paper, Ryan and Ziebland say that in general the researchers did
not ask about the role of companion animals. Even when people talked about their
pets, it was often not followed up within the interview.
They say, “The topic of pets was almost
exclusively raised in the interview by the participants rather than the
researchers. Variously, pets were physically removed from the interview setting
(by the participant, another member of the household or even the researcher
themselves), written out of the verbatim transcript with an interruption label,
and positioned as irrelevant or not interesting through a lack of engagement by
researchers, who largely failed to prompt participants about the role pets
played in their lives. The pets were rarely mentioned in the analysis and
initial writing up of findings.”
This is despite the fact that one advantage
of qualitative interview research is the opportunity to follow up things the
participant mentions. This suggests the researchers had not considered the role
of pets when designing their interviews, and did not realize during the
interview that it was something worth further questioning, although some
exceptions are included.
Photo: Creatista/Shutterstock |
In some cases, pets played a central role in people’s accounts. For example, one woman described noticing her mother’s dementia first when she realized the dog was no longer being taken care of. In another case, the dog’s expectations of someone’s presence are discussed in relation to the loss of a parent. Pets are mentioned in ways that show they are part of the family, as when having to ensure their safety before taking someone to casualty, or how the pet helps in coping with loneliness after a loss.
When one person’s partner was in hospital for an operation, they said, “but I came home and took the dog for a walk and we had a chat! And we talked this thing through.”
When one person’s partner was in hospital for an operation, they said, “but I came home and took the dog for a walk and we had a chat! And we talked this thing through.”
The paper is based on what’s called a ‘secondary analysis’ of an existing dataset of interviews. They looked at 231 interviews with people with autism, Parkinson’s disease, stroke or heart failure, and carers of people with dementia or multiple sclerosis. The interviews were all conducted as part of research into aspects of those conditions – and notably the focus was not on pets.
The transcripts were studied for places where pets appeared. Anywhere that an ‘interruption’ was listed they went to the audio recordings to see what happened. In some cases the interruption was a pet, in which case they transcribed this in full. Then they analyzed all the places where pets were mentioned or made a noise.
Some of the interruptions are distractions
from the research process, as when a participant stopped to go and get some
biscuits for the dog, or the researcher tries to interact with a dog which is
then frightened. At other times, references were made to important roles the
pet plays as companion, confidant, and support.
We shouldn’t get too hung up on the one
researcher who was too distracted by a cat to listen to the participant in that
moment. The real issue is that across the whole set of interviews, pets were
often ignored even when they were relevant to the topic of the study. It is a
reminder that health researchers should be aware that pets might come up in
ways relevant to the research and that weren’t anticipated.
Despite the omission of pets from the
original published papers, in other ways this study shows qualitative research
at its best. Not only were the written transcripts available, but they could be
checked against the audio from the interviews, and the original researchers
made themselves available to answer questions, such as when more context was needed.
In other words, the audit trail worked exactly as it is supposed to, and the
secondary analysis is excellent.
The accounts highlighted in this paper show
people talking about their pets as family members, in a manner that recognizes
that others may not see animals in the same way. Although the paper focuses on
health research, this is a topic with wider relevance to the role of animals in
society.
Do you consider your pets to be family
members?
Reference
Ryan, S., & Ziebland, S. (2015). On interviewing people with pets: reflections from qualitative research on people with long-term conditions Sociology of Health & Illness, 37 (1), 67-80
Ryan, S., & Ziebland, S. (2015). On interviewing people with pets: reflections from qualitative research on people with long-term conditions Sociology of Health & Illness, 37 (1), 67-80
You might also like:
Are dogs good for our health?
My dog comes first: The importance of pets to homeless youth
Cats and their owners are more in sync than you think
My dog comes first: The importance of pets to homeless youth
Cats and their owners are more in sync than you think