How Does a Dog's Brain Respond to the Smell of a Familiar Human?
And what does it tell us about the importance of people to
their dogs?
By Zazie Todd, PhD
This page contains affiliate links which means I may earn a commission on qualifying purchases at no cost to you.
How do you know you have a special place in your dog’s heart?
If you enjoyed this, you might also like:
Dogs Can Haz BrainScanz and EEG?
Canine Neuroscience
As an Amazon Associate I earn from qualifying purchases.
Photo: hitmanphoto / Shutterstock |
By Zazie Todd, PhD
This page contains affiliate links which means I may earn a commission on qualifying purchases at no cost to you.
New fMRI research by Gregory Berns et al (in press) shows that
dog’s brains respond differently to the smell of a familiar human compared to an
unfamiliar human and other canines – suggesting that certain people are special
to their dogs.
The research focussed on a part of the brain called the
caudate, which has been much investigated in humans, monkeys and rats. The
scientists explain that “caudate activity is correlated with salient, usually
rewarding signals that cause the animal to change its behavioural orientation
to approach or consume the stimulus.”
Previous research by the team showed that this part of the brain lights up when the dog is given a hand signal that means it will be given a treat, confirming that caudate activation in dogs is connected with rewards.
The results showed that the caudate was activated significantly more in response to the smell of the familiar human than to any of the other smells – even the familiar dog. The scientists say,
Previous research by the team showed that this part of the brain lights up when the dog is given a hand signal that means it will be given a treat, confirming that caudate activation in dogs is connected with rewards.
The results showed that the caudate was activated significantly more in response to the smell of the familiar human than to any of the other smells – even the familiar dog. The scientists say,
“Importantly, the scent of the familiar human was not the handler, meaning that the caudate response differentiated the scent in the absence of the person being present. The caudate activation suggested that not only did the dogs discriminate that scent from the others, they had a positive association with it. This speaks to the power of the dog’s sense of smell, and it provides clues to the importance of humans in dog’s lives.”
Does this mean we can say that dogs love us? It’s certainly
the case that when people look at photographs of loved ones, the same part of
the brain is activated. But it's hard to interpret the activation on the scan in terms of the dog's subjective experience.
The researchers caution there is another possible
explanation in terms of conditioning. It may be that the familiar person had
previously given the dog food and so the scent was simply eliciting a
conditioned response. The researchers say they think it
unlikely it is a conditioned response, because it was typically the handler –
not the familiar human – who was responsible for feeding the dog.
The results also showed that the olfactory bulb in the brain
was activated by all five smells. This is not surprising but it is useful to
know the result is as expected. The canine brain presents a bit of a challenge
for fMRI studies – training needs aside – simply because of the great variety
of head shapes in dogs.
12 dogs took part in the study. They had all previously
taken part in fMRI research, in which they had to lie absolutely still during
the scan. The smells came from swabs taken from the armpit of humans and from
the perineal-genital area of dogs.
The scents used in the study were of a familiar human, an
unfamiliar human, a familiar dog, an unfamiliar dog, and the dog’s own scent.
The familiar human was not the dog’s main caregiver – as that person was
present during the scan – but someone else from the household, typically the
husband or child of the main caregiver. The familiar dog lived in the same
house.
The dogs were trained using positive reinforcement and models
of the equipment. A clicker was used in
initial stages of the training, but since the equipment is noisy it would not
be heard during the scan itself. The dogs were taught a hand signal that meant
they would get a reward, and this was used to replace a clicker in later stages
of training.
The training specific to this study included preparing the
dog for a different head-coil than in previous scans, and getting used to
having scent-impregnated cotton wool swabs put under the nose while they
remained still.
The number of dogs is small, and there are always trade-offs
in the statistics used to make sense of fMRI scans. But the results are very
intriguing, and we look forward to future research from this team.
The full paper is available (open access) at the link below. Photographs of the dogs who took part are on page 3. You might also be interested to read about subsequent research from this team on dogs' preferences for food and social interaction.
The full paper is available (open access) at the link below. Photographs of the dogs who took part are on page 3. You might also be interested to read about subsequent research from this team on dogs' preferences for food and social interaction.
If you liked this post, check out my book Wag: The Science of Making Your Dog Happy. Modern Dog magazine calls it "The must-have guide to improving your dog's life."
How do you know you have a special place in your dog’s heart?
Reference
Berns, G., Brooks, A., & Spivak, M. (2014). Scent of the familiar: An fMRI study of canine brain responses to familiar and unfamiliar human and dog odors Behavioural Processes DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2014.02.011If you enjoyed this, you might also like:
Dogs Can Haz BrainScanz and EEG?
Canine Neuroscience
As an Amazon Associate I earn from qualifying purchases.