Do Dogs Get that Eureka! Feeling?
Does successful problem solving make dogs happy, or does simply getting the reward make them happy too?
By Zazie Todd, PhD
This page contains affiliate links which means I may earn a commission on qualifying purchases at no cost to you.
References
Photo: Mackland (Shutterstock) |
By Zazie Todd, PhD
This page contains affiliate links which means I may earn a commission on qualifying purchases at no cost to you.
New research by Ragen McGowan et al (University of
Agricultural Sciences, Sweden) investigates whether dogs enjoy the experience
of solving a problem in order to obtain a reward, or if it is just the reward
itself that makes them happy.
Rather unusually, the idea came from a study that found cattle
who completed a task to earn a reward seemed to be happier than those who just
received the reward. The design of McGowan et al’s study is very similar, but
adjusted for dogs.
The results show that when dogs solved the problem and
earned a reward they wagged their tails more and were more eager to repeat the
experience than if they were just given a reward. The study also found that
food was a preferred reward over time with another dog and petting from a
familiar human.
Six matched pairs of beagles took part (12 dogs in total). Each
dog was an experimental dog for half of the time, and a control dog for the
other half of the time.
The study used six pieces of equipment. When manipulated
correctly by the dog, each made a distinct noise that would show the task was
complete. The equipment included a dog piano that had to be pressed to play a
note, a plastic box to be pushed off a stack so it would noisily hit the floor,
and a paddle lever that would make a bell ring.
Before the experiment, each dog was trained on three of the
pieces of equipment while their matched pair was trained on the other three.
The paired piece of equipment was present during training sessions, but the dog
did not get rewards for interacting with it.
In the experiment itself, both pieces of equipment were
again present, but the situation was new.
The experimental room had a start arena with the equipment,
and a gate to a runway that led to the reward. An experimenter was hidden away,
ready to open the gate at the appropriate time. An assistant led the dog into
the start arena, then turned their back and did not interact with them further.
When the experimental dog performed the behaviour it had been trained to do,
the gate opened to give access to the ramp leading to the reward.
When it was the dog’s turn to be a control, it did not
matter what it did with the equipment. The gate opened after the length of time
it had taken the matched experimental dog to solve the puzzle, so the dog spent
the exact same amount of time in the start arena as their pair. They also got
the exact same reward their pair had obtained. In other words, the only
difference between the conditions was whether or not their manipulation of the
equipment would have an effect on the gate opening.
The experimental dogs were keen to get to the start arena
and usually went into the room ahead of the assistant. On the other hand, the
scientists noticed that the control dogs “were initially eager to enter the
room during their first two or three test runs, but soon grew reluctant to
enter the test room. By the end of the test sessions, these dogs would enter
the start arena only after some coaxing from the handler.”
There were other signs that dogs in the control condition
were less happy than those in the experimental condition. They were less active
in the start arena. They would sometimes bite or chew on the equipment, which
dogs never did in the experimental condition. Once the gate had opened, they
were quicker to enter the runway and leave the start arena than dogs in the
experimental condition. There were no differences in mean heart rate, however.
There was more tail wagging when dogs were in the
experimental condition, which also suggests they were happier.
When the reward was food or time with another dog, the
control dogs were quicker to exit the start arena. There was no difference
between experimental and control dogs if the reward was petting by a human they
knew. But dogs in both conditions were more active when expecting a food
reward, suggesting this was preferred. This is consistent with earlier studies
that found dogs prefer food to petting (Okamoto et al 2009; Fukuzawa and Hayashi 2013; Feuerbacher and Wynne (2012), who also
looked at hand-reared wolves)
When dogs were controls, they still received a reward but
they did not have any control over their environment. Perceived lack of control
is stressful for humans, so perhaps it is for dogs. In fact towards the end of the study, some of the
dogs were successfully manipulating the item they had not been trained on, although
of course this had no effect on the gate.
In the experimental condition,
however, dogs were able to solve the problem and make the gate open. I am not sure if it is problem-solving, the
fact it gave them control, or a combination of both that made dogs happy.
The researchers say, “The experimental animals in our study
were excited not only by the expectation of a reward, but also about realizing
that they themselves could control their access to the reward. These results
support the idea that opportunities to solve problems, make decisions, and
exercise cognitive skills are important to an animal’s emotional experiences
and ultimately, its welfare.”
Because differences in behaviour were found in the start
arena, they relate to the dog learning that it can open the gate by
manipulating the equipment. The scientists call it a “Eureka moment”.
If you liked this post, check out my book Wag: The Science of Making Your Dog Happy. Modern Dog magazine calls it "The must-have guide to improving your dog's life."
What kind of opportunities do you give your dog to
problem-solve and make decisions in his or her everyday life?
References
Feuerbacher, E. N., & Wynne, C. D.
(2012). Relative efficacy of human social interaction and food as
reinforcers for domestic dogs and hand‐reared wolves. Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior, 98(1), 105-129.
Fukuzawa, M., & Hayashi, N. (2013). Comparison of 3 different reinforcements of learning in dogs (Canis familiaris). Journal of veterinary behavior, 8(4), 221-224.
McGowan, R. T., Rehn, T., Norling, Y.,
& Keeling, L. J. (2014). Positive affect and learning: exploring
the “Eureka Effect” in dogs. Animal cognition, 17, 577-587.
Okamoto, Y., Ohtani, N., Uchiyama, H.,
& Ohta, M. (2009). The feeding behavior of dogs correlates with
their responses to commands. Journal of Veterinary Medical Science, 71(12), 1617-1621.
As an Amazon Associate I earn from qualifying purchases.