Dogs can haz brainscanz and EEG?
Dogs have been trained to take part in non-invasive EEG and fMRI studies, with no sedation or restraint, just the power of positive reinforcement.
Canine cognition is a hot topic these days, using
experiments and brain imaging as research tools. The trouble with brain imaging
work is that it is invasive, to the extent that animals may have to be
sedated or anaesthetized for the study. All that changed with the
amazing work of Gregory Berns et al and the first-ever fMRI study on awake, unrestrained dogs last year. Now
Miiamaaria Kujala et al in Finland have shown that it is also possible to do a non-invasive
EEG with dogs.
An EEG measures brain activity by placing electrodes across
the scalp. These pick up oscillations in electrical activity, which can be
measured for changes. One common use of EEG is in assessing epilepsy in dogs
(and people). We aren’t talking about veterinary EEGs here, however, but those
designed to learn something about how a healthy brain works.
If animals have to be anaesthetized for an EEG to occur, it’s
a problem because a drowsy brain does not function in the same way as an alert
brain. Awake animals are typically restrained. For example, Hanlu Ma et al (2013) anaesthetized
cats and surgically implanted metal tubes through which electrodes could be inserted. After the cats were given a couple of
weeks to recover from surgery, the electrodes were used to test the cats’
responses to meows and to human voices making vowel sounds.
The cat’s body was wrapped in a cotton bag and its head was
immobilized while the sounds were played. The cats were trained for this (though the paper doesn't say how) and monitored for signs of distress. The results showed which parts of the brain were
activated, and found no significant difference in response to meows and vowels.
In this study, the cats were awake. But it is still invasive,
since they had to be operated on and were restrained for several hours at a time. Could there be another
way?
Since dogs are easily trainable using operant conditioning, Kujala
et al in Finland thought it might be possible to train dogs for EEG. Using
positive reinforcement, they trained eight beagles to take part in their study.
The beagles were purpose-bred for laboratory work and live
in a group kennel environment. First of all they took part in training. For the
study, their heads had to be shaved, cleaned and prepped so that electrodes
could be applied. They wore seven electrodes on the head, one in each ear, and
a ground electrode on the back. Then they had to lie still and look at a TV
screen while measurements were taken. At the same time, they also wore eye-tracking
equipment.
The experiment itself took place in twenty-minute sessions over
four days for each dog, so that they did not get too tired. Of course, it took
much longer to train the dogs to get used to the laboratory and the equipment
in the first place, with twice-weekly training sessions over one and a half years.
A beagle in the study. Source: PLoS One |
The dogs were shown photographs of human and dog faces,
mostly the right way up but with some upside-down. They were shown a batch of
photos, then had a short break in which they were rewarded with some food, then
led to settle down and watch another batch. The authors point out that the
experimental set-up is very similar to that used in human studies.
The results showed a change in a type of electrical activity
called the beta range (15-30Hz); oscillations in this band were suppressed when
the dog was looking at a face, compared to the rest period. This probably
reflects the activity of a part of the brain called the occipital cortex. In
addition, the researchers found a suppression of activity at the 2-6Hz range.
This coincided with the beginning of looking at an image, and was noticed most
in the sensors at the front of the head. The authors say this may relate to eye
movements as the dog looks at an image that has just appeared on the TV.
There were individual differences between the dogs which is
not surprising, as this is also the case for humans.
The authors conclude that “the study opens the possibility
to implement cognitive neuroscience studies with dogs and to examine the evolutionary
background and divergence of brain function associated with cognition.”
This is similar to the study by Gregory Berns et al that was published last year. They trained two dogs – Callie the rescue feist and McKenzie the agility-loving border collie – to take part in an fMRI. They began training the dogs using a mock-up of the equipment before moving on to the real version. After two months, they were able to take part in the fMRI study. Each dog had to keep absolutely still; if they moved by as little as 3mm, it would make the data useless.
This is similar to the study by Gregory Berns et al that was published last year. They trained two dogs – Callie the rescue feist and McKenzie the agility-loving border collie – to take part in an fMRI. They began training the dogs using a mock-up of the equipment before moving on to the real version. After two months, they were able to take part in the fMRI study. Each dog had to keep absolutely still; if they moved by as little as 3mm, it would make the data useless.
The picture shows Callie during a training session (A) and McKenzie during the study itself (B).
The study found that the reward centre of the brain lit up
when the dog saw a hand signal that meant a treat would soon be forthcoming.
These EEG and fMRI studies are a tremendous achievement on
the part of both the humans and dogs that took part. So how were the dogs
trained? They did not use electric shocks or ‘corrections’ or punishment.
Instead they relied on positive reinforcement. (You will have noticed ongoing positive
reinforcement in the EEG study, with pauses in which the dog was given a treat
before returning to the experiment).
These two studies were designed to find out about the canine
brain, but they also show the effectiveness of training using positive reinforcement.
Some people (even some dog trainers) try to argue that positive
reinforcement is not the right way to train a dog. And yet, it has been used to train dogs to take part in an EEG study and in
fMRI without the need for sedation or restraint. Isn’t that amazing?!
References
Berns, G. S., Brooks, A. M., & Spivak, M. (2012). Functional MRI in awake unrestrained dogs. PloS one, 7(5), e38027.
Kujala, M. V., Törnqvist, H., Somppi, S., Hänninen, L., Krause, C. M., Vainio, O., & Kujala, J. (2013). Reactivity of dogs' brain oscillations to visual stimuli measured with non-invasive electroencephalography. PLoS One, 8(5), e61818.
Ma, H., Qin, L., Dong, C., Zhong, R., & Sato, Y. (2013). Comparison of neural responses to cat meows and human vowels in the anterior and posterior auditory field of awake cats. PloS one, 8(1), e52942.
As an Amazon Associate I earn from qualifying purchases. As an Etsy affiliate and Marks and Spencer affiliate, I earn from qualifying purchases.
Ma, H., Qin, L., Dong, C., Zhong, R., & Sato, Y. (2013). Comparison of neural responses to cat meows and human vowels in the anterior and posterior auditory field of awake cats. PloS one, 8(1), e52942.
As an Amazon Associate I earn from qualifying purchases. As an Etsy affiliate and Marks and Spencer affiliate, I earn from qualifying purchases.